----- Original Message -----
Sent: Monday, October 14, 2002 8:49 AM
Subject: Article on Synesthesia in today's Washington
Post
Of
special interest to Nabokovians will be the interview with a local professor of
Russian literature who has synesthesia and like VV, sees different colours for
the Cyrilic alphabet and the Roman alphabet.
/Alphonse Vinh
washingtonpost.com
When
Sound Is Red: Making Sense of Mixed Sensations
By
Shankar Vedantam
Washington Post Staff Writer
Monday, October 14, 2002;
Page A12
Joanne
Innis was around 5 years old when she asked her mother, "How come if you and
Aunt Pat are sisters, you're red and
she's brown?"
When
Glenda Larcombe hears a truck backing up, making a beep-beep-beep sound, she
"sees" the beeps as a series of red
dots.
And
when psychologist Thomas Palmeri gives one of his test subjects a difficult test
-- to spot a tiny "2" on a computer screen
scattered with tiny "5s" -- the
man finds it instantly: To him, the "2" shows up bathed in a different
color.
These
are all examples of synesthesia, an unusual phenomenon whereby people experience
different senses blending into one
another. Some synesthetes experience
individual words in particular colors. Others experience smells when exposed to
shapes
or hear sounds inside tastes.
While
most experts do not consider it a disorder -- synesthetes are usually glad to
have the ability, and it sharply improves their
memory -- research into
synesthesia is teaching scientists important lessons about the normal brain,
perhaps even about aspects
of creativity.
"Synesthesia is seven times more common among artists, novelists and
poets," said Vilayanur Ramachandran, a neurologist at
the University of
California at San Diego, who is researching the phenomenon. "What do artists
have in common? They have
the ability to link seemingly unconnected
domains."
Ramachandran thinks that the power of metaphor and the blending of
realities that artists strive for are phenomena that
synesthetes experience
all the time. While that is currently only a hypothesis, it is certainly true
that synesthetes seem to
experience the world with more intensity -- what
scholars call "affect." However, many of them don't realize they have a
unique
ability, believing that everyone else experiences the same sensations,
too.
While
superficially resembling a drug-induced hallucination, synesthesia feels
profoundly normal to synesthetes. After Innis, an
assistant professor of
Russian at Goucher College in Baltimore, realized that she saw the world
differently than most people,
she understood why she was never interested in
experimenting with drugs like LSD: "There was too much going on in my
head
already," she quipped.
Various explanations have been offered for synesthesia, and while there
are tantalizing clues and plausible theories, no one has
yet identified a
gene or found a neurotransmitter responsible for it.
One
theory is that synesthesia may be caused by "cross-wiring" between areas of the
brain that process different sensations.
Palmeri says his research at
Vanderbilt University has ruled out cross-wiring at least in some areas of the
cortex through
experiments that show subjects different pictures through each
eye. Ramachandran says synesthesia likely arises from
connections in the
fusiform gyrus of the temporal lobe. He expects that scientists will eventually
find a gene or genes that cause
"leakage" of information between disparate
parts of the brain, given that synesthesia seems to run in
families.
Another theory is that everybody may be born with synesthesia, that
infants may experience the world as a jumble of
interwoven sensations and
their different senses may slowly grow distinct, like lenses being brought into
focus. Carol Mills, a
psychologist at Goucher College, says synesthesia might
also be a normal part of all adult brains -- with synesthetes at one end
of a
spectrum.
"It
may go on in all of us even if we don't have synesthesia," said Mills, who
published a paper last week based on Innis in the
journal Perception. "For
example, if I give you a very high-pitched note and a series of colors and ask
you to match one, you
are going to pick a light color. If I give you a low
bass note, you are probably going to pick a dark color. [The difference
is]
when a synesthete hears a low note, they see dark. When they hear a high
note, they see a light color."
The
mingling of senses is often difficult for synesthetes to describe. Larcombe, for
instance, an electronics technician at the
Navy base at Dam Neck in Virginia
Beach, said the red dots she sees when she hears beeping are not part of her
actual vision.
"It's not like I would see a red dot right in front of me --
it's in my mind's eye," she said in an interview. She also reported
"feeling"
her interviewer's voice, "like a wave, like water, with yellow and
orange."
Richard Cytowic, a Washington neurologist and the author of a book about
synesthesia called "The Man Who Tasted Shapes,"
described the case of a
dinner host who told him the chicken didn't have "enough points" on
it.
While
a minority of synesthesias are unpleasant, he said -- like vile-tasting words,
musically induced nausea or billboard colors
out of whack with the
synesthete's internal color scheme -- many synesthetes report intense pleasures
at trivial tasks.
"Remembering someone's phone number is delightful; balancing a checkbook
is delicious," Cytowic said in an interview. "It's
also a rule of thumb that
exceptional talents come at a cost. [Synesthetes] often have trouble with
arithmetic, right-left
orientation and finding
directions."
No
firm figures exist for how common synesthesia is; the best estimates range from
1 in 200 to 1 in 2,000. The most common
forms of synesthesia link numbers or
letters with colors. Even within this group, there are variations of type and
intensity. Innis,
for instance, associates words with the color of the first
letter. Her question to her mother about her aunt Pat was because she
saw the
"M" in "mom" as red and the "P" in her aunt's name as brown.
Innis
said she could also "turn on" the individual colors of every letter in a word,
an especially useful trait when she was learning
Russian in high school.
Mills's research with Innis has explored the unusual fact that Innis not only
has synesthesia for English
words using the Roman script, but for Russian
words in Cyrillic. Mills has determined that Innis's Cyrillic letters drew
their
colors from their English counterparts.
Innis
said she used to have trouble remembering Russian words that start with "o,"
because the letter in her mind was an
unremarkable transparent whitish gray.
So she opened up such words into their constituent colors. For instance,
she
distinguished "ostavit," which means "to leave," from "ostanovitsja,"
which means "to remain," by homing in on the letter "n,"
which occurs only in
the latter word.
"The
'n' gives me a bright orange to stand out," she said, confiding that she avoids
mentioning her special ability to her students,
who have to learn the Russian
words the hard way.
¿ 2002 The Washington Post Company